Leaving Time

    by

    Picoult, Jodi

    “Leaving Time” by Jodi Picoult is a gripping novel that intertwines mystery, grief, and the bond between humans and elephants. The story follows Jenna Metcalf, a 13-year-old girl searching for her mother, Alice, a renowned elephant researcher who disappeared a decade earlier under mysterious circumstances. With the help of a skeptical psychic and a disgraced detective, Jenna uncovers buried secrets about her mother’s work and the tragic events at an elephant sanctuary. The novel explores themes of memory, loss, and maternal love, while weaving in fascinating insights into elephant behavior and emotions. Picoult blends emotional depth with suspense, culminating in a surprising twist that redefines the narrative.

    The chap­ter explores the com­plex emo­tion­al respons­es of ele­phants when encoun­ter­ing the remains of their own species, par­tic­u­lar­ly focus­ing on whether they can dis­tin­guish between bones of famil­iar indi­vid­u­als ver­sus strangers. Obser­va­tions of ele­phants in the wild have shown clear signs of grief—silence, phys­i­cal droop­ing, and gen­tle touching—when they come across ele­phant bones. How­ev­er, sci­en­tif­ic exper­i­ments con­duct­ed in Amboseli, Kenya, aimed to test this fur­ther by pre­sent­ing ele­phants with var­i­ous objects, includ­ing ivory, skulls, and wood, to mea­sure their reac­tions and pref­er­ences.

    Researchers con­duct­ed con­trolled exper­i­ments with ele­phant herds, intro­duc­ing items like ivory frag­ments, ele­phant skulls, and non-ele­phant skulls to observe behav­ioral dif­fer­ences. The results showed ele­phants were most drawn to ivory, fol­lowed by ele­phant skulls, sug­gest­ing a species-spe­cif­ic recog­ni­tion. When pre­sent­ed with skulls of ele­phants, rhi­nos, and buf­fa­lo, the ele­phants con­sis­tent­ly pri­or­i­tized ele­phant remains, indi­cat­ing a clear pref­er­ence for their own kind. This rein­forced the idea that ele­phants have a unique con­nec­tion to their species’ remains.

    A deep­er lay­er of the study involved pre­sent­ing skulls of deceased matri­archs to herds that had known them. Con­trary to expec­ta­tions, the ele­phants did not show a stronger reac­tion to their own for­mer lead­ers’ skulls com­pared to oth­ers. This raised ques­tions about whether ele­phants tru­ly grieve spe­cif­ic indi­vid­u­als or sim­ply respond to ele­phant remains in gen­er­al. While ear­li­er anec­dotes sug­gest­ed deep, per­son­al mourn­ing, the exper­i­men­tal results seemed to chal­lenge this, leav­ing room for debate about the nature of ele­phant emo­tions.

    The chap­ter con­cludes by reflect­ing on the impli­ca­tions of these find­ings. While the study demon­strates ele­phants’ fas­ci­na­tion with their species’ bones, it also com­pli­cates the under­stand­ing of ele­phant grief. The absence of pref­er­en­tial behav­ior toward famil­iar remains might sug­gest that ele­phants mourn ele­phants as a col­lec­tive rather than as indi­vid­u­als. Alter­na­tive­ly, it could imply that the loss of any matriarch—regardless of per­son­al connection—holds uni­ver­sal sig­nif­i­cance, hint­ing at a broad­er, com­mu­nal sense of loss among ele­phants.

    FAQs

    • 1. What were the key findings from the Amboseli elephant experiments regarding their interaction with bones and ivory?

      Answer:
      The Amboseli experiments revealed several important findings about elephant behavior. When presented with a small piece of ivory, an elephant skull, and a block of wood, elephants showed the most interest in the ivory—stroking, carrying, and rolling it beneath their feet. In another test with elephant, rhino, and water buffalo skulls, elephants focused most on the elephant skull. However, when presented with skulls of deceased matriarchs from their own and other herds, elephants showed equal interest in all three skulls, suggesting they may not distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar elephant remains despite their clear preference for elephant bones over other objects.

      2. How does the chapter challenge common assumptions about elephant grief and memory?

      Answer:
      The chapter challenges the assumption that elephants specifically mourn individual elephants they knew well. While anecdotal observations (like those in Botswana) suggest elephants display grief for companions, the controlled experiments showed no preference for skulls of their own matriarchs over others. This raises questions about whether elephants grieve individuals or respond more generally to elephant remains. The author proposes an alternative interpretation: that elephants may value all mothers equally, implying a broader sense of kinship rather than individualized mourning.

      3. Analyze the significance of the ivory preference in the first experiment. What might this suggest about elephant behavior?

      Answer:
      The elephants’ strong response to ivory—handling it more than skulls or wood—hints at its ecological or social importance. Ivory may carry sensory or symbolic value, perhaps because it resembles tusks used for communication, tool-making, or dominance displays. This behavior could reflect curiosity, a tactile connection to their species’ identity, or even unconscious recognition of ivory as a marker of elephanthood. Unlike the skull experiments, the ivory test underscores elephants’ ability to discriminate between materials, emphasizing their complex sensory awareness.

      4. Critical Thinking: Why might the author conclude that “all mothers are” important based on the skull experiment results?

      Answer:
      The author’s conclusion reframes the data through a lens of collective empathy. If elephants didn’t prioritize their own matriarch’s skull, it might indicate they perceive motherhood as a universal value rather than a personal bond. This interpretation aligns with elephants’ matriarchal social structures, where leadership and care extend beyond immediate family. The author implies that grief or reverence isn’t limited to individual relationships but may honor the broader role of matriarchs in elephant society—a poignant commentary on shared loss and respect.

      5. Application: How could these findings influence conservation efforts or ethical treatment of elephants?

      Answer:
      Understanding elephants’ responses to remains could shape conservation strategies. For instance, if elephants are drawn to ivory, preserving tusks in protected areas might foster engagement or memorial behaviors. The mixed results about individual grief suggest that while elephants may not mourn specific individuals as humans do, their collective response to elephant remains underscores their social complexity. This could advocate for policies that minimize trauma during herd disruptions (e.g., poaching or relocation) and emphasize protecting family units to maintain their intricate social bonds.

    Quotes

    • 1. “Anyone who has ever seen elephants come across the bones of another individual would recognize the calling card of grief: the intense silence, the droop of the trunk and ears, the hesitant caresses, the sadness that seems to wrap the herd like a shroud when they encounter the remains of one of their own.”

      This opening observation establishes elephants’ capacity for grief and mourning, setting up the chapter’s central exploration of elephant cognition and emotional intelligence. The vivid description makes their grief behaviors tangible to human readers.

      2. “Without a doubt, the tiny piece of ivory was the most intriguing to the elephants, followed by the skull and then the wood. They stroked the ivory, picked it up, carried it, rolled it beneath their hind feet.”

      This quote presents key experimental findings showing elephants’ clear preference for elephant remains over other objects. The specific behaviors described demonstrate their fascination with and reverence for elephant remains.

      3. “You’d think that the elephants would have been most interested in the skull that belonged to the matriarch who had led their own herd… But that’s not what happened. Instead, the Amboseli elephants were equally attracted to the three skulls.”

      This surprising experimental result challenges assumptions about elephants’ ability to recognize specific individuals’ remains, creating tension in the chapter’s argument about elephant cognition and memory.

      4. “Although the study proves that elephants are fascinated by the bones of other elephants, some might say it also proves that an elephant experiencing grief for an individual must be a fiction.”

      This statement captures the potential counterargument to the chapter’s thesis, acknowledging how the experimental results could be interpreted to diminish claims about elephants’ emotional capacities.

      5. “But maybe it means that all mothers are.”

      This poignant closing line offers an alternative interpretation of the findings, suggesting elephants may mourn all mothers universally rather than just their own. It provides a powerful, emotionally resonant conclusion to the chapter’s exploration of grief and connection.

    Quotes

    1. “Anyone who has ever seen elephants come across the bones of another individual would recognize the calling card of grief: the intense silence, the droop of the trunk and ears, the hesitant caresses, the sadness that seems to wrap the herd like a shroud when they encounter the remains of one of their own.”

    This opening observation establishes elephants’ capacity for grief and mourning, setting up the chapter’s central exploration of elephant cognition and emotional intelligence. The vivid description makes their grief behaviors tangible to human readers.

    2. “Without a doubt, the tiny piece of ivory was the most intriguing to the elephants, followed by the skull and then the wood. They stroked the ivory, picked it up, carried it, rolled it beneath their hind feet.”

    This quote presents key experimental findings showing elephants’ clear preference for elephant remains over other objects. The specific behaviors described demonstrate their fascination with and reverence for elephant remains.

    3. “You’d think that the elephants would have been most interested in the skull that belonged to the matriarch who had led their own herd… But that’s not what happened. Instead, the Amboseli elephants were equally attracted to the three skulls.”

    This surprising experimental result challenges assumptions about elephants’ ability to recognize specific individuals’ remains, creating tension in the chapter’s argument about elephant cognition and memory.

    4. “Although the study proves that elephants are fascinated by the bones of other elephants, some might say it also proves that an elephant experiencing grief for an individual must be a fiction.”

    This statement captures the potential counterargument to the chapter’s thesis, acknowledging how the experimental results could be interpreted to diminish claims about elephants’ emotional capacities.

    5. “But maybe it means that all mothers are.”

    This poignant closing line offers an alternative interpretation of the findings, suggesting elephants may mourn all mothers universally rather than just their own. It provides a powerful, emotionally resonant conclusion to the chapter’s exploration of grief and connection.

    FAQs

    1. What were the key findings from the Amboseli elephant experiments regarding their interaction with bones and ivory?

    Answer:
    The Amboseli experiments revealed several important findings about elephant behavior. When presented with a small piece of ivory, an elephant skull, and a block of wood, elephants showed the most interest in the ivory—stroking, carrying, and rolling it beneath their feet. In another test with elephant, rhino, and water buffalo skulls, elephants focused most on the elephant skull. However, when presented with skulls of deceased matriarchs from their own and other herds, elephants showed equal interest in all three skulls, suggesting they may not distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar elephant remains despite their clear preference for elephant bones over other objects.

    2. How does the chapter challenge common assumptions about elephant grief and memory?

    Answer:
    The chapter challenges the assumption that elephants specifically mourn individual elephants they knew well. While anecdotal observations (like those in Botswana) suggest elephants display grief for companions, the controlled experiments showed no preference for skulls of their own matriarchs over others. This raises questions about whether elephants grieve individuals or respond more generally to elephant remains. The author proposes an alternative interpretation: that elephants may value all mothers equally, implying a broader sense of kinship rather than individualized mourning.

    3. Analyze the significance of the ivory preference in the first experiment. What might this suggest about elephant behavior?

    Answer:
    The elephants’ strong response to ivory—handling it more than skulls or wood—hints at its ecological or social importance. Ivory may carry sensory or symbolic value, perhaps because it resembles tusks used for communication, tool-making, or dominance displays. This behavior could reflect curiosity, a tactile connection to their species’ identity, or even unconscious recognition of ivory as a marker of elephanthood. Unlike the skull experiments, the ivory test underscores elephants’ ability to discriminate between materials, emphasizing their complex sensory awareness.

    4. Critical Thinking: Why might the author conclude that “all mothers are” important based on the skull experiment results?

    Answer:
    The author’s conclusion reframes the data through a lens of collective empathy. If elephants didn’t prioritize their own matriarch’s skull, it might indicate they perceive motherhood as a universal value rather than a personal bond. This interpretation aligns with elephants’ matriarchal social structures, where leadership and care extend beyond immediate family. The author implies that grief or reverence isn’t limited to individual relationships but may honor the broader role of matriarchs in elephant society—a poignant commentary on shared loss and respect.

    5. Application: How could these findings influence conservation efforts or ethical treatment of elephants?

    Answer:
    Understanding elephants’ responses to remains could shape conservation strategies. For instance, if elephants are drawn to ivory, preserving tusks in protected areas might foster engagement or memorial behaviors. The mixed results about individual grief suggest that while elephants may not mourn specific individuals as humans do, their collective response to elephant remains underscores their social complexity. This could advocate for policies that minimize trauma during herd disruptions (e.g., poaching or relocation) and emphasize protecting family units to maintain their intricate social bonds.

    Note