Cover of Leaving Time
    DramaLiterary Fiction

    Leaving Time

    by Picoult, Jodi
    “Leaving Time” by Jodi Picoult is a gripping novel that intertwines mystery, grief, and the bond between humans and elephants. The story follows Jenna Metcalf, a 13-year-old girl searching for her mother, Alice, a renowned elephant researcher who disappeared a decade earlier under mysterious circumstances. With the help of a skeptical psychic and a disgraced detective, Jenna uncovers buried secrets about her mother’s work and the tragic events at an elephant sanctuary. The novel explores themes of memory, loss, and maternal love, while weaving in fascinating insights into elephant behavior and emotions. Picoult blends emotional depth with suspense, culminating in a surprising twist that redefines the narrative.

    The chap­ter explores the pro­found under­stand­ing of death among ele­phants, high­light­ing their unique griev­ing rit­u­als. Unlike humans, ele­phants do not con­cep­tu­al­ize after­life but expe­ri­ence grief pure­ly as loss. They show no inter­est in the bones of oth­er species but exhib­it rev­er­ence toward deceased ele­phants, even long after death. When encoun­ter­ing ele­phant remains, they approach cau­tious­ly, touch­ing and smelling the bones with their trunks and feet, some­times car­ry­ing them or rock­ing gen­tly over frag­ments. This behav­ior sug­gests a deep, instinc­tu­al recog­ni­tion of their own kind.

    Ele­phant death rit­u­als are well-doc­u­ment­ed by researchers, under­scor­ing their con­sis­ten­cy across obser­va­tions. The chap­ter cites George Adamson’s account of ele­phants relo­cat­ing the bones of a shot bull ele­phant back to its death site. Oth­er renowned researchers, includ­ing Cyn­thia Moss and Joyce Poole, have record­ed sim­i­lar behav­iors. The nar­ra­tor, like­ly a field researcher, aligns their own obser­va­tions with these find­ings, empha­siz­ing the uni­ver­sal­i­ty of ele­phant mourn­ing prac­tices. These rit­u­als reflect a com­mu­nal response to death, tran­scend­ing indi­vid­ual herds.

    A poignant exam­ple involves the matri­arch Bon­tle, whose death is wit­nessed by the nar­ra­tor in Botswana. Her herd’s imme­di­ate reaction—attempting to lift her, mount­ing her, and the calf’s des­per­ate attempts to revive her—reveals their dis­tress. Once Bon­tle dies, the ele­phants cov­er her body with leaves and dirt, stand­ing vig­il for days. Years lat­er, the herd still paus­es at the site of her remains, par­tic­u­lar­ly her skull. The calf, now grown, inter­acts with the skull as if rec­og­niz­ing his moth­er, sug­gest­ing long-term mem­o­ry and emo­tion­al attach­ment.

    The chap­ter con­cludes by rein­forc­ing the idea that ele­phants pos­sess a sophis­ti­cat­ed aware­ness of death, marked by rit­u­als that mir­ror human mourn­ing in their solem­ni­ty and per­sis­tence. Their actions—covering the body, return­ing to remains, and dis­play­ing qui­et reverence—speak to an emo­tion­al depth that chal­lenges sim­plis­tic views of ani­mal cog­ni­tion. The narrator’s first­hand account lends cred­i­bil­i­ty to these obser­va­tions, invit­ing read­ers to recon­sid­er the bound­aries of grief and mem­o­ry in the ani­mal king­dom.

    FAQs

    • 1. How do elephants demonstrate their understanding of death, according to the chapter?

      Answer:
      Elephants show their comprehension of death through distinct mourning behaviors. When encountering elephant remains, they approach cautiously in groups, touching and caressing the bones with their trunks and feet in a reverent manner. They may carry tusks or bones, or gently rock small ivory fragments underfoot. The chapter cites multiple examples, including a herd covering their deceased matriarch Bontle with leaves and dirt, standing vigil for days, and returning years later to pay respects to her scattered bones. These rituals indicate elephants recognize death as a significant event, particularly when it involves their own kind.

      2. What evidence suggests elephants recognize individual remains, not just general elephant bones?

      Answer:
      The chapter provides compelling evidence through the behavior of Bontle’s calf, Kgosi. Years after Bontle’s death, the now-adult male elephant specifically interacted with her skull by placing his trunk where her mouth once was—a gesture mirroring how calves greet their mothers. This targeted behavior implies personal recognition beyond general reverence for elephant remains. Additionally, the herd’s repeated visits to Bontle’s exact death site and their earlier attempts to revive her (including young males mounting her) demonstrate they perceived her as an individual, not just a conspecific carcass.

      3. How does the chapter contrast human and elephant perspectives on death?

      Answer:
      The author distinguishes that while humans often conceptualize death through abstract planning or religious afterlife beliefs, elephants experience grief as a direct response to loss. Their rituals focus on physical remains rather than metaphysical concepts, emphasizing sensory engagement—touching, smelling, and transporting bones. However, the chapter elevates elephant mourning by showing its complexity: they curate death sites (as with the relocated bones in Kenya), maintain long-term memory of deceased individuals, and exhibit behaviors (like covering bodies) that parallel human funerary practices. This suggests their understanding, while different from ours, is deeply emotional and socially significant.

      4. Why is the account of Bontle’s death particularly significant for understanding elephant cognition?

      Answer:
      Bontle’s case study is pivotal because it documents the full progression of elephant responses to death—from immediate attempts to revive her (lifting with tusks, mounting), to organized post-death rituals (covering her body), to sustained multi-year memorial behaviors. The herd’s coordinated actions reveal advanced social cognition, including teaching younger members to mourn (as seen with Kgosi). The prolonged vigil—interrupted only for necessities—also demonstrates intentionality. These observations, combined with citations from renowned researchers, position elephant mourning as a sophisticated, culturally transmitted practice rather than instinct alone.

    Quotes

    • 1. “There is no question that elephants understand death. They may not plan for it the way we do; they may not imagine elaborate afterlives like those in our religious doctrines. For them, grief is simpler, cleaner. It’s all about loss.”

      This opening statement establishes the chapter’s central theme - elephants’ profound yet uncomplicated understanding of mortality. It contrasts human and elephant perspectives on death, framing the subsequent observations.

      2. “Elephants are not particularly interested in the bones of other dead animals, just other elephants… They approach the carcass as a group, and caress the bones with what can only be described as reverence.”

      This quote demonstrates elephants’ species-specific mourning rituals, highlighting their unique behavioral responses to death. The description of their “reverence” suggests an emotional depth comparable to human practices.

      3. “The herd stood solemnly with Bontle’s body for two and a half days, leaving only to get water or food, and then returning.”

      This observation of prolonged vigil behavior provides concrete evidence of elephant grief. The duration and dedication shown mirror human mourning customs, reinforcing the chapter’s argument about elephants’ emotional intelligence.

      4. “Clearly these bones had general significance to him. But if you had seen it, I think you’d believe what I do: that he recognized that these particular bones had once been his mother.”

      This powerful conclusion suggests elephants may have long-term memory and recognition capabilities regarding deceased relatives. The personal account lends credibility to the scientific observations while creating an emotional connection with readers.

    Quotes

    1. “There is no question that elephants understand death. They may not plan for it the way we do; they may not imagine elaborate afterlives like those in our religious doctrines. For them, grief is simpler, cleaner. It’s all about loss.”

    This opening statement establishes the chapter’s central theme - elephants’ profound yet uncomplicated understanding of mortality. It contrasts human and elephant perspectives on death, framing the subsequent observations.

    2. “Elephants are not particularly interested in the bones of other dead animals, just other elephants… They approach the carcass as a group, and caress the bones with what can only be described as reverence.”

    This quote demonstrates elephants’ species-specific mourning rituals, highlighting their unique behavioral responses to death. The description of their “reverence” suggests an emotional depth comparable to human practices.

    3. “The herd stood solemnly with Bontle’s body for two and a half days, leaving only to get water or food, and then returning.”

    This observation of prolonged vigil behavior provides concrete evidence of elephant grief. The duration and dedication shown mirror human mourning customs, reinforcing the chapter’s argument about elephants’ emotional intelligence.

    4. “Clearly these bones had general significance to him. But if you had seen it, I think you’d believe what I do: that he recognized that these particular bones had once been his mother.”

    This powerful conclusion suggests elephants may have long-term memory and recognition capabilities regarding deceased relatives. The personal account lends credibility to the scientific observations while creating an emotional connection with readers.

    FAQs

    1. How do elephants demonstrate their understanding of death, according to the chapter?

    Answer:
    Elephants show their comprehension of death through distinct mourning behaviors. When encountering elephant remains, they approach cautiously in groups, touching and caressing the bones with their trunks and feet in a reverent manner. They may carry tusks or bones, or gently rock small ivory fragments underfoot. The chapter cites multiple examples, including a herd covering their deceased matriarch Bontle with leaves and dirt, standing vigil for days, and returning years later to pay respects to her scattered bones. These rituals indicate elephants recognize death as a significant event, particularly when it involves their own kind.

    2. What evidence suggests elephants recognize individual remains, not just general elephant bones?

    Answer:
    The chapter provides compelling evidence through the behavior of Bontle’s calf, Kgosi. Years after Bontle’s death, the now-adult male elephant specifically interacted with her skull by placing his trunk where her mouth once was—a gesture mirroring how calves greet their mothers. This targeted behavior implies personal recognition beyond general reverence for elephant remains. Additionally, the herd’s repeated visits to Bontle’s exact death site and their earlier attempts to revive her (including young males mounting her) demonstrate they perceived her as an individual, not just a conspecific carcass.

    3. How does the chapter contrast human and elephant perspectives on death?

    Answer:
    The author distinguishes that while humans often conceptualize death through abstract planning or religious afterlife beliefs, elephants experience grief as a direct response to loss. Their rituals focus on physical remains rather than metaphysical concepts, emphasizing sensory engagement—touching, smelling, and transporting bones. However, the chapter elevates elephant mourning by showing its complexity: they curate death sites (as with the relocated bones in Kenya), maintain long-term memory of deceased individuals, and exhibit behaviors (like covering bodies) that parallel human funerary practices. This suggests their understanding, while different from ours, is deeply emotional and socially significant.

    4. Why is the account of Bontle’s death particularly significant for understanding elephant cognition?

    Answer:
    Bontle’s case study is pivotal because it documents the full progression of elephant responses to death—from immediate attempts to revive her (lifting with tusks, mounting), to organized post-death rituals (covering her body), to sustained multi-year memorial behaviors. The herd’s coordinated actions reveal advanced social cognition, including teaching younger members to mourn (as seen with Kgosi). The prolonged vigil—interrupted only for necessities—also demonstrates intentionality. These observations, combined with citations from renowned researchers, position elephant mourning as a sophisticated, culturally transmitted practice rather than instinct alone.

    Note