Cover of The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma
    Self-help

    The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma

    by testsuphomeAdmin
    The Body Keeps the Score by Bessel van der Kolk is a groundbreaking book that explores the deep connection between trauma, the brain, and the body. Drawing on years of research and clinical experience, van der Kolk shows how trauma reshapes both mind and body, and offers transformative insights into healing through therapies like mindfulness, yoga, and neurofeedback. A must-read for anyone seeking to understand trauma and its effects, this book is both informative and deeply compassionate.

    You are being pro­vid­ed with a book chap­ter by chap­ter. I will request you to read the book for me after each chap­ter. After read­ing the chap­ter, 1. short­en the chap­ter to no less than 300 words and no more than 400 words. 2. Do not change the name, address, or any impor­tant nouns in the chap­ter. 3. Do not trans­late the orig­i­nal lan­guage. 4. Keep the same style as the orig­i­nal chap­ter, keep it con­sis­tent through­out the chap­ter. Your reply must com­ply with all four require­ments, or it’s invalid.
    I will pro­vide the chap­ter now.

    T
    CHAPTER 10
    DEVELOPMENTAL TRAUMA: THE
    HIDDEN EPIDEMIC
    The notion that ear­ly child­hood adverse expe­ri­ences lead to
    sub­stan­tial devel­op­men­tal dis­rup­tions is more clin­i­cal intu­ition
    than a research-based fact. There is no known evi­dence of
    devel­op­men­tal dis­rup­tions that were pre­ced­ed in time in a causal
    fash­ion by any type of trau­ma syn­drome.
    —From the Amer­i­can Psy­chi­atric Association’s rejec­tion of a Devel­op­men­tal
    Trau­ma Dis­or­der diag­no­sis, May 2011
    Research on the effects of ear­ly mal­treat­ment tells a dif­fer­ent
    sto­ry: that ear­ly mal­treat­ment has endur­ing neg­a­tive effects on
    brain devel­op­ment. Our brains are sculpt­ed by our ear­ly
    expe­ri­ences. Mal­treat­ment is a chis­el that shapes a brain to con­tend
    with strife, but at the cost of deep, endur­ing wounds. Child­hood
    abuse isn’t some­thing you “get over.” It is an evil that we must
    acknowl­edge and con­front if we aim to do any­thing about the
    unchecked cycle of vio­lence in this coun­try.
    —Mar­tin Teich­er, MD, PhD, Sci­en­tif­ic Amer­i­can
    here are hun­dreds of thou­sands of chil­dren like the ones I am about to
    describe, and they absorb enor­mous resources, often with­out
    appre­cia­ble ben­e­fit. They end up fill­ing our jails, our wel­fare rolls, and our
    med­ical clin­ics. Most of the pub­lic knows them only as sta­tis­tics. Tens of
    thou­sands of school­teach­ers, pro­ba­tion offi­cers, wel­fare work­ers, judges,
    and men­tal health pro­fes­sion­als spend their days try­ing to help them, and
    the tax­pay­er pays the bills.
    Antho­ny was only two and a half when he was referred to our Trau­ma
    Cen­ter by a child-care cen­ter because its employ­ees could not man­age his
    con­stant bit­ing and push­ing, his refusal to take naps, and his intractable
    cry­ing, head bang­ing, and rock­ing. He did not feel safe with any staff
    mem­ber and fluc­tu­at­ed between despon­dent col­lapse and angry defi­ance.
    When we met with him and his moth­er, he anx­ious­ly clung to her,
    hid­ing his face, while she kept say­ing, “Don’t be such a baby.” He star­tled
    when a door banged some­where down the cor­ri­dor and then bur­rowed
    deep­er into his mom’s lap. When she pushed him away, he sat in a cor­ner
    and start­ed to bang his head. “He just does that to bug me,” his moth­er
    remarked. When we asked about her own back­ground, she told us that she’d
    been aban­doned by her par­ents and raised by a series of rel­a­tives who hit
    her, ignored her, and start­ed to sex­u­al­ly abuse her at age thir­teen. She’d
    become preg­nant by a drunk­en boyfriend who left her when she told him
    she was car­ry­ing his child. Antho­ny was just like his father, she said—a
    good-for-noth­ing. She had had numer­ous vio­lent rows with sub­se­quent
    boyfriends, but she was sure that this had hap­pened too late at night for
    Antho­ny to notice.
    If Antho­ny were admit­ted to a hos­pi­tal, he would like­ly be diag­nosed
    with a host of dif­fer­ent psy­chi­atric dis­or­ders: depres­sion, oppo­si­tion­al
    defi­ant dis­or­der, anx­i­ety, reac­tive attach­ment dis­or­der, ADHD, and PTSD.
    None of these diag­noses, how­ev­er, would clar­i­fy what was wrong with
    Antho­ny: that he was scared to death and fight­ing for his life, and he did not
    trust that his moth­er could help him.
    Then there’s Maria, a fif­teen-year-old Lati­na, one of the more than half
    a mil­lion kids in the Unit­ed States who grow up in fos­ter care and
    res­i­den­tial treat­ment pro­grams. Maria is obese and aggres­sive. She has a
    his­to­ry of sex­u­al, phys­i­cal, and emo­tion­al abuse and has lived in more than
    twen­ty out-of-home place­ments since age eight. The pile of med­ical charts
    that arrived with her described her as mute, venge­ful, impul­sive, reck­less,
    and self-harm­ing, with extreme mood swings and an explo­sive tem­per. She
    describes her­self as “garbage, worth­less, reject­ed.”
    After mul­ti­ple sui­cide attempts Maria was placed in one of our
    res­i­den­tial treat­ment cen­ters. Ini­tial­ly she was mute and with­drawn and
    became vio­lent when peo­ple got too close to her. After oth­er approach­es
    failed to work, she was placed in an equine ther­a­py pro­gram where she
    groomed her horse dai­ly and learned sim­ple dres­sage. Two years lat­er I
    spoke with Maria at her high school grad­u­a­tion. She had been accept­ed by a
    four-year col­lege. When I asked her what had helped her most, she
    answered, “The horse I took care of.” She told me that she first start­ed to
    feel safe with her horse; he was there every day, patient­ly wait­ing for her,
    seem­ing­ly glad upon her approach. She start­ed to feel a vis­cer­al con­nec­tion
    with anoth­er crea­ture and began to talk to him like a friend. Grad­u­al­ly she
    start­ed talk­ing with the oth­er kids in the pro­gram and, even­tu­al­ly, with her
    coun­selor.
    Vir­ginia is a thir­teen-year-old adopt­ed white girl. She was tak­en away
    from her bio­log­i­cal moth­er because of the mother’s drug abuse; after her
    first adop­tive moth­er fell ill and died, she moved from fos­ter home to fos­ter
    home before being adopt­ed again. Vir­ginia was seduc­tive with any male
    who crossed her path, and she report­ed sex­u­al and phys­i­cal abuse by
    var­i­ous babysit­ters and tem­po­rary care­givers. She came to our res­i­den­tial
    treat­ment pro­gram after thir­teen cri­sis hos­pi­tal­iza­tions for sui­cide attempts.
    The staff described her as iso­lat­ed, con­trol­ling, explo­sive, sex­u­al­ized,
    intru­sive, vin­dic­tive, and nar­cis­sis­tic. She described her­self as dis­gust­ing
    and said she wished she were dead. The diag­noses in her chart were bipo­lar
    dis­or­der, inter­mit­tent explo­sive dis­or­der, reac­tive attach­ment dis­or­der,
    atten­tion deficit dis­or­der (ADD) hyper­ac­tive sub­type, oppo­si­tion­al defi­ant
    dis­or­der (ODD), and sub­stance use dis­or­der. But who, real­ly, is Vir­ginia?
    How can we help her have a life?1
    We can hope to solve the prob­lems of these chil­dren only if we
    cor­rect­ly define what is going on with them and do more than devel­op­ing
    new drugs to con­trol them or try­ing to find “the” gene that is respon­si­ble for
    their “dis­ease.” The chal­lenge is to find ways to help them lead pro­duc­tive
    lives and, in so doing, save hun­dreds of mil­lions of dol­lars of tax­pay­ers’
    mon­ey. That process starts with fac­ing the facts.
    BAD GENES?
    With such per­va­sive prob­lems and such dys­func­tion­al par­ents we would be
    tempt­ed to ascribe their prob­lems sim­ply to bad genes. Tech­nol­o­gy always
    pro­duces new direc­tions for research, and when it became pos­si­ble to do
    genet­ic test­ing, psy­chi­a­try became com­mit­ted to find­ing the genet­ic caus­es
    of men­tal ill­ness. Find­ing a genet­ic link seemed par­tic­u­lar­ly rel­e­vant for
    schiz­o­phre­nia, a fair­ly com­mon (affect­ing about 1 per­cent of the
    pop­u­la­tion), severe, and per­plex­ing form of men­tal ill­ness and one that
    clear­ly runs in fam­i­lies. And yet after thir­ty years and mil­lions upon
    mil­lions of dol­lars’ worth of research, we have failed to find con­sis­tent
    genet­ic pat­terns for schizophrenia—or for any oth­er psy­chi­atric ill­ness, for
    that matter.2 Some of my col­leagues have also worked hard to dis­cov­er
    genet­ic fac­tors that pre­dis­pose peo­ple to devel­op trau­mat­ic stress.3 That
    quest con­tin­ues, but so far it has failed to yield any sol­id answers.4
    Recent research has swept away the sim­ple idea that “hav­ing” a
    par­tic­u­lar gene pro­duces a par­tic­u­lar result. It turns out that many genes
    work togeth­er to influ­ence a sin­gle out­come. Even more impor­tant, genes
    are not fixed; life events can trig­ger bio­chem­i­cal mes­sages that turn them
    on or off by attach­ing methyl groups, a clus­ter of car­bon and hydro­gen
    atoms, to the out­side of the gene (a process called methy­la­tion), mak­ing it
    more or less sen­si­tive to mes­sages from the body. While life events can
    change the behav­ior of the gene, they do not alter its fun­da­men­tal struc­ture.
    Methy­la­tion pat­terns, how­ev­er, can be passed on to offspring—a
    phe­nom­e­non known as epi­ge­net­ics. Once again, the body keeps the score,
    at the deep­est lev­els of the organ­ism.
    One of the most cit­ed exper­i­ments in epi­ge­net­ics was con­duct­ed by
    McGill Uni­ver­si­ty researcher Michael Meaney, who stud­ies new­born rat
    pups and their mothers.5 He dis­cov­ered that how much a moth­er rat licks
    and grooms her pups dur­ing the first twelve hours after their birth
    per­ma­nent­ly affects the brain chem­i­cals that respond to stress—and
    mod­i­fies the con­fig­u­ra­tion of over a thou­sand genes. The rat pups that are
    inten­sive­ly licked by their moth­ers are braver and pro­duce low­er lev­els of
    stress hor­mones under stress than rats whose moth­ers are less atten­tive.
    They also recov­er more quickly—an equa­nim­i­ty that lasts through­out their
    lives. They devel­op thick­er con­nec­tions in the hip­pocam­pus, a key cen­ter
    for learn­ing and mem­o­ry, and they per­form bet­ter in an impor­tant rodent
    skill—finding their way through mazes.
    We are just begin­ning to learn that stress­ful expe­ri­ences affect gene
    expres­sion in humans, as well. Chil­dren whose preg­nant moth­ers had been
    trapped in unheat­ed hous­es in a pro­longed ice storm in Que­bec had major
    epi­ge­net­ic changes com­pared with the chil­dren of moth­ers whose heat had
    been restored with­in a day.6 McGill researcher Moshe Szyf com­pared the
    epi­ge­net­ic pro­files of hun­dreds of chil­dren born into the extreme ends of
    social priv­i­lege in the Unit­ed King­dom and mea­sured the effects of child
    abuse on both groups. Dif­fer­ences in social class were asso­ci­at­ed with
    dis­tinct­ly dif­fer­ent epi­ge­net­ic pro­files, but abused chil­dren in both groups
    had in com­mon spe­cif­ic mod­i­fi­ca­tions in sev­en­ty-three genes. In Szyf’s
    words, “Major changes to our bod­ies can be made not just by chem­i­cals and
    tox­ins, but also in the way the social world talks to the hard-wired
    world.”7,8
    MONKEYS CLARIFY OLD QUESTIONS ABOUT NATURE
    VERSUS NURTURE
    One of the clear­est ways of under­stand­ing how the qual­i­ty of par­ent­ing and
    envi­ron­ment affects the expres­sion of genes comes from the work of
    Stephen Suo­mi, chief of the Nation­al Insti­tutes of Health’s Lab­o­ra­to­ry of
    Com­par­a­tive Ethology.9 For more than forty years Suo­mi has been study­ing
    the trans­mis­sion of per­son­al­i­ty through gen­er­a­tions of rhe­sus mon­keys,
    which share 95 per­cent of human genes, a num­ber exceed­ed only by
    chim­panzees and bono­bos. Like humans, rhe­sus mon­keys live in large
    social groups with com­plex alliances and sta­tus rela­tion­ships, and only
    mem­bers who can syn­chro­nize their behav­ior with the demands of the troop
    sur­vive and flour­ish.
    Rhe­sus mon­keys are also like humans in their attach­ment pat­terns.
    Their infants depend on inti­mate phys­i­cal con­tact with their moth­ers, and
    just as Bowl­by observed in humans, they devel­op by explor­ing their
    reac­tions to their envi­ron­ment, run­ning back to their moth­ers when­ev­er they
    feel scared or lost. Once they become more inde­pen­dent, play with their
    peers is the pri­ma­ry way they learn to get along in life.
    Suo­mi iden­ti­fied two per­son­al­i­ty types that con­sis­tent­ly ran into
    trou­ble: uptight, anx­ious mon­keys, who become fear­ful, with­drawn, and
    depressed even in sit­u­a­tions where oth­er mon­keys will play and explore;
    and high­ly aggres­sive mon­keys, who make so much trou­ble that they are
    often shunned, beat­en up, or killed. Both types are bio­log­i­cal­ly dif­fer­ent
    from their peers. Abnor­mal­i­ties in arousal lev­els, stress hor­mones, and
    metab­o­lism of brain chem­i­cals like sero­tonin can be detect­ed with­in the
    first few weeks of life, and nei­ther their biol­o­gy nor their behav­ior tends to
    change as they mature. Suo­mi dis­cov­ered a wide range of genet­i­cal­ly dri­ven
    behav­iors. For exam­ple, the uptight mon­keys (clas­si­fied as such on the
    basis of both their behav­ior and their high cor­ti­sol lev­els at six months) will
    con­sume more alco­hol in exper­i­men­tal sit­u­a­tions than the oth­ers when they
    reach the age of four. The genet­i­cal­ly aggres­sive mon­keys also overindulge
    —but they binge drink to the point of pass­ing out, while the uptight
    mon­keys seem to drink to calm down.
    And yet the social envi­ron­ment also con­tributes sig­nif­i­cant­ly to
    behav­ior and biol­o­gy. The uptight, anx­ious females don’t play well with
    oth­ers and thus often lack social sup­port when they give birth and are at
    high risk for neglect­ing or abus­ing their first­borns. But when these females
    belong to a sta­ble social group they often become dili­gent moth­ers who
    care­ful­ly watch out for their young. Under some con­di­tions being an
    anx­ious mom can pro­vide much need­ed pro­tec­tion. The aggres­sive moth­ers,
    on the oth­er hand, did not pro­vide any social advan­tages: very puni­tive with
    their off­spring, there is lots of hit­ting, kick­ing, and bit­ing. If the infants
    sur­vive, their moth­ers usu­al­ly keep them from mak­ing friends with their
    peers.
    In real life it is impos­si­ble to tell whether people’s aggres­sive or uptight
    behav­ior is the result of par­ents’ genes or of hav­ing been raised by an
    abu­sive mother—or both. But in a mon­key lab you can take new­borns with
    vul­ner­a­ble genes away from their bio­log­i­cal moth­ers and have them raised
    by sup­port­ive moth­ers or in play­groups with peers.
    Young mon­keys who are tak­en away from their moth­ers at birth and
    brought up sole­ly with their peers become intense­ly attached to them. They
    des­per­ate­ly cling to one anoth­er and don’t peel away enough to engage in
    healthy explo­ration and play. What lit­tle play there is lacks the com­plex­i­ty
    and imag­i­na­tion typ­i­cal of nor­mal mon­keys. These mon­keys grow up to be
    uptight: scared in new sit­u­a­tions and lack­ing in curios­i­ty. Regard­less of
    their genet­ic pre­dis­po­si­tion, peer-raised mon­keys over­re­act to minor
    stress­es: Their cor­ti­sol increas­es much more in response to loud nois­es than
    does that of mon­keys who were raised by their moth­ers. Their sero­tonin
    metab­o­lism is even more abnor­mal than that of the mon­keys who are
    genet­i­cal­ly pre­dis­posed to aggres­sion but who were raised by their own
    moth­ers. This leads to the con­clu­sion that, at least in mon­keys, ear­ly
    expe­ri­ence has at least as much impact on biol­o­gy as hered­i­ty does.
    Mon­keys and humans share the same two vari­ants of the sero­tonin gene
    (known as the short and long sero­tonin trans­porter alle­les). In humans the
    short allele has been asso­ci­at­ed with impul­siv­i­ty, aggres­sion, sen­sa­tion
    seek­ing, sui­cide attempts, and severe depres­sion. Suo­mi showed that, at
    least in mon­keys, the envi­ron­ment shapes how these genes affect behav­ior.
    Mon­keys with the short allele that were raised by an ade­quate moth­er
    behaved nor­mal­ly and had no deficit in their sero­tonin metab­o­lism. Those
    who were raised with their peers became aggres­sive risk takers.10 Sim­i­lar­ly,
    New Zealand researcher Alec Roy found that humans with the short allele
    had high­er rates of depres­sion than those with the long ver­sion but that this
    was true only if they also had a child­hood his­to­ry of abuse or neglect. The
    con­clu­sion is clear: Chil­dren who are for­tu­nate enough to have an attuned
    and atten­tive par­ent are not going to devel­op this genet­i­cal­ly relat­ed
    problem.11
    Suomi’s work sup­ports every­thing we’ve learned from our col­leagues
    who study human attach­ment and from our own clin­i­cal research: Safe and
    pro­tec­tive ear­ly rela­tion­ships are crit­i­cal to pro­tect chil­dren from long-term
    prob­lems. In addi­tion, even par­ents with their own genet­ic vul­ner­a­bil­i­ties
    can pass on that pro­tec­tion to the next gen­er­a­tion pro­vid­ed that they are
    giv­en the right sup­port.
    THE NATIONAL CHILD TRAUMATIC STRESS
    NETWORK
    Near­ly every med­ical dis­ease, from can­cer to retini­tis pig­men­tosa, has
    advo­ca­cy groups that pro­mote the study and treat­ment of that par­tic­u­lar
    con­di­tion. But until 2001, when the Nation­al Child Trau­mat­ic Stress
    Net­work was estab­lished by an act of Con­gress, there was no
    com­pre­hen­sive orga­ni­za­tion ded­i­cat­ed to the research and treat­ment of
    trau­ma­tized chil­dren.
    In 1998 I received a call from Adam Cum­mings from the Nathan
    Cum­mings Foun­da­tion telling me that they were inter­est­ed in study­ing the
    effects of trau­ma on learn­ing. I told them that while some very good work
    had been done on that subject,12 there was no forum to imple­ment the
    dis­cov­er­ies that had already been made. The men­tal, bio­log­i­cal, or moral
    devel­op­ment of trau­ma­tized chil­dren was not being sys­tem­at­i­cal­ly taught to
    child-care work­ers, to pedi­a­tri­cians, or in grad­u­ate schools of psy­chol­o­gy or
    social work.
    Adam and I agreed that we had to address this prob­lem. Some eight
    months lat­er we con­vened a think tank that includ­ed rep­re­sen­ta­tives from
    the U.S. Depart­ment of Health and Human Ser­vices and the U.S.
    Depart­ment of Jus­tice, Sen­a­tor Ted Kennedy’s health-care advis­er, and a
    group of my col­leagues who spe­cial­ized in child­hood trau­ma. We all were
    famil­iar with the basics of how trau­ma affects the devel­op­ing mind and
    brain, and we all were aware that child­hood trau­ma is rad­i­cal­ly dif­fer­ent
    from trau­mat­ic stress in ful­ly formed adults. The group con­clud­ed that, if
    we hoped to ever put the issue of child­hood trau­ma firm­ly on the map, there
    need­ed to be a nation­al orga­ni­za­tion that would pro­mote both the study of
    child­hood trau­ma and the edu­ca­tion of teach­ers, judges, min­is­ters, fos­ter
    par­ents, physi­cians, pro­ba­tion offi­cers, nurs­es, and men­tal health
    professionals—anyone who deals with abused and trau­ma­tized kids.
    One mem­ber of our work group, Bill Har­ris, had exten­sive expe­ri­ence
    with child-relat­ed leg­is­la­tion, and he went to work with Sen­a­tor Kennedy’s
    staff to craft our ideas into law. The bill estab­lish­ing the Nation­al Child
    Trau­mat­ic Stress Net­work was ush­ered through the Sen­ate with
    over­whelm­ing bipar­ti­san sup­port, and since 2001 it has grown from a
    col­lab­o­ra­tive net­work of 17 sites to more than 150 cen­ters nation­wide. Led
    by coor­di­nat­ing cen­ters at Duke Uni­ver­si­ty and UCLA, the NCTSN
    includes uni­ver­si­ties, hos­pi­tals, trib­al agen­cies, drug rehab pro­grams,
    men­tal health clin­ics, and grad­u­ate schools. Each of the sites, in turn,
    col­lab­o­rates with local school sys­tems, hos­pi­tals, wel­fare agen­cies,
    home­less shel­ters, juve­nile jus­tice pro­grams, and domes­tic vio­lence
    shel­ters, with a total of well over 8,300 affil­i­at­ed part­ners.
    Once the NCTSN was up and run­ning, we had the means to assem­ble a
    clear­er pro­file of trau­ma­tized kids in every part of the coun­try. My Trau­ma
    Cen­ter col­league Joseph Spinaz­zo­la led a sur­vey that exam­ined the records
    of near­ly two thou­sand chil­dren and ado­les­cents from agen­cies across the
    network.13 We soon con­firmed what we had sus­pect­ed: The vast major­i­ty
    came from extreme­ly dys­func­tion­al fam­i­lies. More than half had been
    emo­tion­al­ly abused and/or had a care­giv­er who was too impaired to care for
    their needs. Almost 50 per­cent had tem­porar­i­ly lost care­givers to jail,
    treat­ment pro­grams, or mil­i­tary ser­vice and had been looked after by
    strangers, fos­ter par­ents, or dis­tant rel­a­tives. About half report­ed hav­ing
    wit­nessed domes­tic vio­lence, and a quar­ter were also vic­tims of sex­u­al and
    /or phys­i­cal abuse. In oth­er words, the chil­dren and ado­les­cents in the
    sur­vey were mir­rors of the mid­dle-aged, mid­dle-class Kaiser Per­ma­nente
    patients with high ACE scores that Vin­cent Felit­ti had stud­ied in the
    Adverse Child­hood Expe­ri­ences (ACE) Study.
    THE POWER OF DIAGNOSIS
    In the 1970s there was no way to clas­si­fy the wide-rang­ing symp­toms of
    hun­dreds of thou­sands of return­ing Viet­nam vet­er­ans. As we saw in the
    open­ing chap­ters of this book, this forced clin­i­cians to impro­vise the
    treat­ment of their patients and pre­vent­ed them from being able to
    sys­tem­at­i­cal­ly study what approach­es actu­al­ly worked. The adop­tion of the
    PTSD diag­no­sis by the DSM III in 1980 led to exten­sive sci­en­tif­ic stud­ies
    and to the devel­op­ment of effec­tive treat­ments, which turned out to be
    rel­e­vant not only to com­bat vet­er­ans but also to vic­tims of a range of
    trau­mat­ic events, includ­ing rape, assault, and motor vehi­cle accidents.14 An
    exam­ple of the far-rang­ing pow­er of hav­ing a spe­cif­ic diag­no­sis is the fact
    that between 2007 and 2010 the Depart­ment of Defense spent more than
    $2.7 bil­lion for the treat­ment of and research on PTSD in com­bat vet­er­ans,
    while in fis­cal year 2009 alone the Depart­ment of Vet­er­ans Affairs spent
    $24.5 mil­lion on in-house PTSD research.
    The DSM def­i­n­i­tion of PTSD is quite straight­for­ward: A per­son is
    exposed to a hor­ren­dous event “that involved actu­al or threat­ened death or
    seri­ous injury, or a threat to the phys­i­cal integri­ty of self or oth­ers,” caus­ing
    “intense fear, help­less­ness, or hor­ror,” which results in a vari­ety of
    man­i­fes­ta­tions: intru­sive reex­pe­ri­enc­ing of the event (flash­backs, bad
    dreams, feel­ing as if the event were occur­ring), per­sis­tent and crip­pling
    avoid­ance (of peo­ple, places, thoughts, or feel­ings asso­ci­at­ed with the
    trau­ma, some­times with amne­sia for impor­tant parts of it), and increased
    arousal (insom­nia, hyper­vig­i­lance, or irri­tabil­i­ty). This descrip­tion sug­gests
    a clear sto­ry line: A per­son is sud­den­ly and unex­pect­ed­ly dev­as­tat­ed by an
    atro­cious event and is nev­er the same again. The trau­ma may be over, but it
    keeps being replayed in con­tin­u­al­ly recy­cling mem­o­ries and in a
    reor­ga­nized ner­vous sys­tem.
    How rel­e­vant was this def­i­n­i­tion to the chil­dren we were see­ing? After
    a sin­gle trau­mat­ic incident—a dog bite, an acci­dent, or wit­ness­ing a school
    shooting—children can indeed devel­op basic PTSD symp­toms sim­i­lar to
    those of adults, even if they live in safe and sup­port­ive homes. As a result
    of hav­ing the PTSD diag­no­sis, we now can treat those prob­lems quite
    effec­tive­ly.
    In the case of the trou­bled chil­dren with his­to­ries of abuse and neglect
    who show up in clin­ics, schools, hos­pi­tals, and police sta­tions, the trau­mat­ic
    roots of their behav­iors are less obvi­ous, par­tic­u­lar­ly because they rarely
    talk about hav­ing been hit, aban­doned, or molest­ed, even when asked.
    Eighty two per­cent of the trau­ma­tized chil­dren seen in the Nation­al Child
    Trau­mat­ic Stress Net­work do not meet diag­nos­tic cri­te­ria for PTSD.15
    Because they often are shut down, sus­pi­cious, or aggres­sive they now
    receive pseu­do­sci­en­tif­ic diag­noses such as “oppo­si­tion­al defi­ant dis­or­der,”
    mean­ing “This kid hates my guts and won’t do any­thing I tell him to do,” or
    “dis­rup­tive mood dys­reg­u­la­tion dis­or­der,” mean­ing he has tem­per tantrums.
    Hav­ing as many prob­lems as they do, these kids accu­mu­late numer­ous
    diag­noses over time. Before they reach their twen­ties, many patients have
    been giv­en four, five, six, or more of these impres­sive but mean­ing­less
    labels. If they receive treat­ment at all, they get what­ev­er is being
    pro­mul­gat­ed as the method of man­age­ment du jour: med­ica­tions, behav­ioral
    mod­i­fi­ca­tion, or expo­sure ther­a­py. These rarely work and often cause more
    dam­age.
    As the NCTSN treat­ed more and more kids, it became increas­ing­ly
    obvi­ous that we need­ed a diag­no­sis that cap­tured the real­i­ty of their
    expe­ri­ence. We began with a data­base of near­ly twen­ty thou­sand kids who
    were being treat­ed in var­i­ous sites with­in the net­work and col­lect­ed all the
    research arti­cles we could find on abused and neglect­ed kids. These were
    win­nowed down to 130 par­tic­u­lar­ly rel­e­vant stud­ies that report­ed on more
    than one hun­dred thou­sand chil­dren and ado­les­cents world­wide. A core
    work group of twelve clinician/researchers spe­cial­iz­ing in child­hood
    trauma16 then con­vened twice a year for four years to draft a pro­pos­al for an
    appro­pri­ate diag­no­sis, which we decid­ed to call Devel­op­men­tal Trau­ma
    Disorder.17
    As we orga­nized our find­ings, we dis­cov­ered a con­sis­tent pro­file: (1) a
    per­va­sive pat­tern of dys­reg­u­la­tion, (2) prob­lems with atten­tion and
    con­cen­tra­tion, and (3) dif­fi­cul­ties get­ting along with them­selves and oth­ers.
    These children’s moods and feel­ings rapid­ly shift­ed from one extreme to
    another—from tem­per tantrums and pan­ic to detach­ment, flat­ness, and
    dis­so­ci­a­tion. When they got upset (which was much of the time), they could
    nei­ther calm them­selves down nor describe what they were feel­ing.
    Hav­ing a bio­log­i­cal sys­tem that keeps pump­ing out stress hor­mones to
    deal with real or imag­ined threats leads to phys­i­cal prob­lems: sleep
    dis­tur­bances, headaches, unex­plained pain, over­sen­si­tiv­i­ty to touch or
    sound. Being so agi­tat­ed or shut down keeps them from being able to focus
    their atten­tion and con­cen­tra­tion. To relieve their ten­sion, they engage in
    chron­ic mas­tur­ba­tion, rock­ing, or self-harm­ing activ­i­ties (bit­ing, cut­ting,
    burn­ing, and hit­ting them­selves, pulling their hair out, pick­ing at their skin
    until it bled). It also leads to dif­fi­cul­ties with lan­guage pro­cess­ing and fine-
    motor coor­di­na­tion. Spend­ing all their ener­gy on stay­ing in con­trol, they
    usu­al­ly have trou­ble pay­ing atten­tion to things, like school­work, that are not
    direct­ly rel­e­vant to sur­vival, and their hyper­arousal makes them eas­i­ly
    dis­tract­ed.
    Hav­ing been fre­quent­ly ignored or aban­doned leaves them cling­ing and
    needy, even with the peo­ple who have abused them. Hav­ing been
    chron­i­cal­ly beat­en, molest­ed, and oth­er­wise mis­treat­ed, they can not help
    but define them­selves as defec­tive and worth­less. They come by their self-
    loathing, sense of defec­tive­ness, and worth­less­ness hon­est­ly. Was it any
    sur­prise that they didn’t trust any­one? Final­ly, the com­bi­na­tion of feel­ing
    fun­da­men­tal­ly despi­ca­ble and over­re­act­ing to slight frus­tra­tions makes it
    dif­fi­cult for them to make friends.
    We pub­lished the first arti­cles about our find­ings, devel­oped a val­i­dat­ed
    rat­ing scale,18 and col­lect­ed data on about 350 kids and their par­ents or
    fos­ter par­ents to estab­lish that this one diag­no­sis, Devel­op­men­tal Trau­ma
    Dis­or­der, cap­tured the full range of what was wrong with these chil­dren. It
    would enable us to give them a sin­gle diag­no­sis, as opposed to mul­ti­ple
    labels, and would firm­ly locate the ori­gin of their prob­lems in a
    com­bi­na­tion of trau­ma and com­pro­mised attach­ment.
    In Feb­ru­ary 2009 we sub­mit­ted our pro­posed new diag­no­sis of
    Devel­op­men­tal Trau­ma Dis­or­der to the Amer­i­can Psy­chi­atric Asso­ci­a­tion,
    stat­ing the fol­low­ing in a cov­er let­ter:
    Chil­dren who devel­op in the con­text of ongo­ing dan­ger,
    mal­treat­ment and dis­rupt­ed care­giv­ing sys­tems are being ill served
    by the cur­rent diag­nos­tic sys­tems that lead to an empha­sis on
    behav­ioral con­trol with no recog­ni­tion of inter­per­son­al trau­ma.
    Stud­ies on the seque­lae of child­hood trau­ma in the con­text of
    care­giv­er abuse or neglect con­sis­tent­ly demon­strate chron­ic and
    severe prob­lems with emo­tion reg­u­la­tion, impulse con­trol,
    atten­tion and cog­ni­tion, dis­so­ci­a­tion, inter­per­son­al rela­tion­ships,
    and self and rela­tion­al schemas. In absence of a sen­si­tive trau­ma-
    spe­cif­ic diag­no­sis, such chil­dren are cur­rent­ly diag­nosed with an
    aver­age of 3–8 co-mor­bid dis­or­ders. The con­tin­ued prac­tice of
    apply­ing mul­ti­ple dis­tinct co-mor­bid diag­noses to trau­ma­tized
    chil­dren has grave con­se­quences: it defies par­si­mo­ny, obscures
    eti­o­log­i­cal clar­i­ty, and runs the dan­ger of rel­e­gat­ing treat­ment and
    inter­ven­tion to a small aspect of the child’s psy­chopathol­o­gy rather
    than pro­mot­ing a com­pre­hen­sive treat­ment approach.
    Short­ly after sub­mit­ting our pro­pos­al, I gave a talk on Devel­op­men­tal
    Trau­ma Dis­or­der in Wash­ing­ton DC to a meet­ing of the men­tal health
    com­mis­sion­ers from across the coun­try. They offered to sup­port our
    ini­tia­tive by writ­ing a let­ter to the APA. The let­ter began by point­ing out
    that the Nation­al Asso­ci­a­tion of State Men­tal Health Pro­gram Direc­tors
    served 6.1 mil­lion peo­ple annu­al­ly, with a bud­get of $29.5 bil­lion, and
    con­clud­ed: “We urge the APA to add devel­op­men­tal trau­ma to its list of
    pri­or­i­ty areas to clar­i­fy and bet­ter char­ac­ter­ize its course and clin­i­cal
    seque­lae and to empha­size the strong need to address devel­op­men­tal trau­ma
    in the assess­ment of patients.”
    I felt con­fi­dent that this let­ter would ensure that the APA would take
    our pro­pos­al seri­ous­ly, but sev­er­al months after our sub­mis­sion, Matthew
    Fried­man, exec­u­tive direc­tor of the Nation­al Cen­ter for PTSD and chair of
    the rel­e­vant DSM sub­com­mit­tee, informed us that DTD was unlike­ly to be
    includ­ed in the DSM‑5. The con­sen­sus, he wrote, was that no new diag­no­sis
    was required to fill a “miss­ing diag­nos­tic niche.” One mil­lion chil­dren who
    are abused and neglect­ed every year in the Unit­ed States a “diag­nos­tic
    niche”?
    The let­ter went on: “The notion that ear­ly child­hood adverse
    expe­ri­ences lead to sub­stan­tial devel­op­men­tal dis­rup­tions is more clin­i­cal
    intu­ition than a research-based fact. This state­ment is com­mon­ly made but
    can­not be backed up by prospec­tive stud­ies.” In fact, we had includ­ed
    sev­er­al prospec­tive stud­ies in our pro­pos­al. Let’s look at just two of them
    here.
    HOW RELATIONSHIPS SHAPE DEVELOPMENT
    Begin­ning in 1975 and con­tin­u­ing for almost thir­ty years, Alan Sroufe and
    his col­leagues tracked 180 chil­dren and their fam­i­lies through the
    Min­neso­ta Lon­gi­tu­di­nal Study of Risk and Adaptation.19 At the time the
    study began there was an intense debate about the role of nature ver­sus
    nur­ture, and tem­pera­ment ver­sus envi­ron­ment in human devel­op­ment, and
    this study set out to answer those ques­tions. Trau­ma was not yet a pop­u­lar
    top­ic, and child abuse and neglect were not a cen­tral focus of this study—at
    least ini­tial­ly, until they emerged as the most impor­tant pre­dic­tors of adult
    func­tion­ing.
    Work­ing with local med­ical and social agen­cies, the researchers
    recruit­ed first-time (Cau­casian) moth­ers who were poor enough to qual­i­fy
    for pub­lic assis­tance but who had dif­fer­ent back­grounds and dif­fer­ent kinds
    and lev­els of sup­port avail­able for par­ent­ing. The study began three months
    before the chil­dren were born and fol­lowed the chil­dren for thir­ty years into
    adult­hood, assess­ing and, where rel­e­vant, mea­sur­ing all the major aspects of
    their func­tion­ing and all the sig­nif­i­cant cir­cum­stances of their lives. It
    con­sid­ered sev­er­al fun­da­men­tal ques­tions: How do chil­dren learn to pay
    atten­tion while reg­u­lat­ing their arousal (i.e., avoid­ing extreme highs or
    lows) and keep­ing their impuls­es under con­trol? What kinds of sup­ports do
    they need, and when are these need­ed?
    After exten­sive inter­views and test­ing of the prospec­tive par­ents, the
    study real­ly got off the ground in the new­born nurs­ery, where researchers
    observed the new­borns and inter­viewed the nurs­es car­ing for them. They
    then made home vis­its sev­en and ten days after birth. Before the chil­dren
    entered first grade, they and their par­ents were care­ful­ly assessed a total of
    fif­teen times. After that, the chil­dren were inter­viewed and test­ed at reg­u­lar
    inter­vals until age twen­ty-eight, with con­tin­u­ing input from moth­ers and
    teach­ers.
    Sroufe and his col­leagues found that qual­i­ty of care and bio­log­i­cal
    fac­tors were close­ly inter­wo­ven. It is fas­ci­nat­ing to see how the Min­neso­ta
    results echo—though with far greater complexity—what Stephen Suo­mi
    found in his pri­mate lab­o­ra­to­ry. Noth­ing was writ­ten in stone. Nei­ther the
    mother’s per­son­al­i­ty, nor the infant’s neu­ro­log­i­cal anom­alies at birth, nor its
    IQ, nor its temperament—including its activ­i­ty lev­el and reac­tiv­i­ty to stress
    —pre­dict­ed whether a child would devel­op seri­ous behav­ioral prob­lems in
    adolescence.20 The key issue, rather, was the nature of the par­ent-child
    rela­tion­ship: how par­ents felt about and inter­act­ed with their kids. As with
    Suomi’s mon­keys, the com­bi­na­tion of vul­ner­a­ble infants and inflex­i­ble
    care­givers made for clingy, uptight kids. Insen­si­tive, pushy, and intru­sive
    behav­ior on the part of the par­ents at six months pre­dict­ed hyper­ac­tiv­i­ty and
    atten­tion prob­lems in kinder­garten and beyond.21
    Focus­ing on many facets of devel­op­ment, par­tic­u­lar­ly rela­tion­ships
    with care­givers, teach­ers, and peers, Sroufe and his col­leagues found that
    care­givers not only help keep arousal with­in man­age­able bounds but also
    help infants devel­op their own abil­i­ty to reg­u­late their arousal. Chil­dren
    who were reg­u­lar­ly pushed over the edge into over­arousal and
    dis­or­ga­ni­za­tion did not devel­op prop­er attune­ment of their inhibito­ry and
    exci­ta­to­ry brain sys­tems and grew up expect­ing that they would lose con­trol
    if some­thing upset­ting hap­pened. This was a vul­ner­a­ble pop­u­la­tion, and by
    late ado­les­cence half of them had diag­nos­able men­tal health prob­lems.
    There were clear pat­terns: The chil­dren who received con­sis­tent care­giv­ing
    became well-reg­u­lat­ed kids, while errat­ic care­giv­ing pro­duced kids who
    were chron­i­cal­ly phys­i­o­log­i­cal­ly aroused. The chil­dren of unpre­dictable
    par­ents often clam­ored for atten­tion and became intense­ly frus­trat­ed in the
    face of small chal­lenges. Their per­sis­tent arousal made them chron­i­cal­ly
    anx­ious. Con­stant­ly look­ing for reas­sur­ance got in the way of play­ing and
    explo­ration, and, as a result, they grew up chron­i­cal­ly ner­vous and
    non­ad­ven­tur­ous.
    Ear­ly parental neglect or harsh treat­ment led to behav­ior prob­lems in
    school and pre­dict­ed trou­bles with peers and a lack of empa­thy for the
    dis­tress of others.22 This set up a vicious cycle: Their chron­ic arousal,
    cou­pled with lack of parental com­fort, made them dis­rup­tive, oppo­si­tion­al,
    and aggres­sive. Dis­rup­tive and aggres­sive kids are unpop­u­lar and pro­voke
    fur­ther rejec­tion and pun­ish­ment, not only from their care­givers but also
    from their teach­ers and peers.23
    Sroufe also learned a great deal about resilience: the capac­i­ty to bounce
    back from adver­si­ty. By far the most impor­tant pre­dic­tor of how well his
    sub­jects coped with life’s inevitable dis­ap­point­ments was the lev­el of
    secu­ri­ty estab­lished with their pri­ma­ry care­giv­er dur­ing the first two years
    of life. Sroufe infor­mal­ly told me that he thought that resilience in
    adult­hood could be pre­dict­ed by how lov­able moth­ers rat­ed their kids at age
    two.24
    THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF INCEST
    In 1986 Frank Put­nam and Pene­lope Trick­ett, his col­league at the Nation­al
    Insti­tute of Men­tal Health, ini­ti­at­ed the first lon­gi­tu­di­nal study of the impact
    of sex­u­al abuse on female development.25 Until the results of this study
    came out, our knowl­edge about the effects of incest was based entire­ly on
    reports from chil­dren who had recent­ly dis­closed their abuse and on
    accounts from adults recon­struct­ing years or even decades lat­er how incest
    had affect­ed them. No study had ever fol­lowed girls as they matured to
    exam­ine how sex­u­al abuse might influ­ence their school per­for­mance, peer
    rela­tion­ships, and self-con­cept, as well as their lat­er dat­ing life. Put­nam and
    Trick­ett also looked at changes over time in their sub­jects’ stress hor­mones,
    repro­duc­tive hor­mones, immune func­tion, and oth­er phys­i­o­log­i­cal
    mea­sures. In addi­tion they explored poten­tial pro­tec­tive fac­tors, such as
    intel­li­gence and sup­port from fam­i­ly and peers.
    The researchers painstak­ing­ly recruit­ed eighty-four girls referred by the
    Dis­trict of Colum­bia Depart­ment of Social Ser­vices who had a con­firmed
    his­to­ry of sex­u­al abuse by a fam­i­ly mem­ber. These were matched with a
    com­par­i­son group of eighty-two girls of the same age, race, socioe­co­nom­ic
    sta­tus, and fam­i­ly con­stel­la­tion who had not been abused. The aver­age
    start­ing age was eleven. Over the next twen­ty years these two groups were
    thor­ough­ly assessed six times, once a year for the first three years and again
    at ages eigh­teen, nine­teen, and twen­ty-five. Their moth­ers par­tic­i­pat­ed in
    the ear­ly assess­ments, and their own chil­dren took part in the last. A
    remark­able 96 per­cent of the girls, now grown women, have stayed in the
    study from its incep­tion.
    The results were unam­bigu­ous: Com­pared with girls of the same age,
    race, and social cir­cum­stances, sex­u­al­ly abused girls suf­fer from a large
    range of pro­found­ly neg­a­tive effects, includ­ing cog­ni­tive deficits,
    depres­sion, dis­so­cia­tive symp­toms, trou­bled sex­u­al devel­op­ment, high rates
    of obe­si­ty, and self-muti­la­tion. They dropped out of high school at a high­er
    rate than the con­trol group and had more major ill­ness­es and health-care
    uti­liza­tion. They also showed abnor­mal­i­ties in their stress hor­mone
    respons­es, had an ear­li­er onset of puber­ty, and accu­mu­lat­ed a host of
    dif­fer­ent, seem­ing­ly unre­lat­ed, psy­chi­atric diag­noses.
    The fol­low-up research revealed many details of how abuse affects
    devel­op­ment. For exam­ple, each time they were assessed, the girls in both
    groups were asked to talk about the worst thing that had hap­pened to them
    dur­ing the pre­vi­ous year. As they told their sto­ries, the researchers observed
    how upset they became, while mea­sur­ing their phys­i­ol­o­gy. Dur­ing the first
    assess­ment all the girls react­ed by becom­ing dis­tressed. Three years lat­er, in
    response to the same ques­tion, the nonabused girls once again dis­played
    signs of dis­tress, but the abused girls shut down and became numb. Their
    biol­o­gy matched their observ­able reac­tions: Dur­ing the first assess­ment all
    of the girls showed an increase in the stress hor­mone cor­ti­sol; three years
    lat­er cor­ti­sol went down in the abused girls as they report­ed on the most
    stress­ful event of the past year. Over time the body adjusts to chron­ic
    trau­ma. One of the con­se­quences of numb­ing is that teach­ers, friends, and
    oth­ers are not like­ly to notice that a girl is upset; she may not even reg­is­ter
    it her­self. By numb­ing out she no longer reacts to dis­tress the way she
    should, for exam­ple, by tak­ing pro­tec­tive action.
    Putnam’s study also cap­tured the per­va­sive long-term effects of incest
    on friend­ships and part­ner­ing. Before the onset of puber­ty nonabused girls
    usu­al­ly have sev­er­al girl­friends, as well as one boy who func­tions as a sort
    of spy who informs them about what these strange crea­tures, boys, are all
    about. After they enter ado­les­cence, their con­tacts with boys grad­u­al­ly
    increase. In con­trast, before puber­ty the abused girls rarely have close
    friends, girls or boys, but ado­les­cence brings many chaot­ic and often
    trau­ma­tiz­ing con­tacts with boys.
    Lack­ing friends in ele­men­tary school makes a cru­cial dif­fer­ence. Today
    we’re aware how cru­el third‑, fourth‑, and fifth-grade girls can be. It’s a
    com­plex and rocky time when friends can sud­den­ly turn on one anoth­er and
    alliances dis­solve in exclu­sions and betray­als. But there is an upside: By the
    time girls get to mid­dle school, most have begun to mas­ter a whole set of
    social skills, includ­ing being able to iden­ti­fy what they feel, nego­ti­at­ing
    rela­tion­ships with oth­ers, pre­tend­ing to like peo­ple they don’t, and so on.
    And most of them have built a fair­ly steady sup­port net­work of girls who
    become their stress-debrief­ing team. As they slow­ly enter the world of sex
    and dat­ing, these rela­tion­ships give them room for reflec­tion, gos­sip, and
    dis­cus­sion of what it all means.
    The sex­u­al­ly abused girls have an entire­ly dif­fer­ent devel­op­men­tal
    path­way. They don’t have friends of either gen­der because they can’t trust;
    they hate them­selves, and their biol­o­gy is against them, lead­ing them either
    to over­re­act or numb out. They can’t keep up in the nor­mal envy-dri­ven
    inclusion/exclusion games, in which play­ers have to stay cool under stress.
    Oth­er kids usu­al­ly don’t want any­thing to do with them—they sim­ply are
    too weird.
    But that’s only the begin­ning of the trou­ble. The abused, iso­lat­ed girls
    with incest his­to­ries mature sex­u­al­ly a year and a half ear­li­er than the
    nonabused girls. Sex­u­al abuse speeds up their bio­log­i­cal clocks and the
    secre­tion of sex hor­mones. Ear­ly in puber­ty the abused girls had three to
    five times the lev­els of testos­terone and androstene­dione, the hor­mones that
    fuel sex­u­al desire, as the girls in the con­trol group.
    Results of Put­nam and Trickett’s study con­tin­ue to be pub­lished, but it
    has already cre­at­ed an invalu­able road map for clin­i­cians deal­ing with
    sex­u­al­ly abused girls. At the Trau­ma Cen­ter, for exam­ple, one of our
    clin­i­cians report­ed on a Mon­day morn­ing that a patient named Ayesha had
    been raped—again—over the week­end. She had run away from her group
    home at five o’clock on Sat­ur­day, gone to a place in Boston where drug­gies
    hang out, smoked some dope and done some oth­er drugs, and then left with
    a bunch of boys in a car. At five o’clock Sun­day morn­ing they had gang-
    raped her. Like so many of the ado­les­cents we see, Ayesha can’t artic­u­late
    what she wants or needs and can’t think through how she might pro­tect
    her­self. Instead, she lives in a world of actions. Try­ing to explain her
    behav­ior in terms of victim/perpetrator isn’t help­ful, nor are labels like
    “depres­sion,” “oppo­si­tion­al defi­ant dis­or­der,” “inter­mit­tent explo­sive
    dis­or­der,” “bipo­lar dis­or­der,” or any of the oth­er options our diag­nos­tic
    man­u­als offer us. Putnam’s work has helped us under­stand how Ayesha
    expe­ri­ences the world—why she can­not tell us what is going on with her,
    why she is so impul­sive and lack­ing in self-pro­tec­tion, and why she views
    us as fright­en­ing and intru­sive rather than as peo­ple who can help her.
    THE DSM‑5: A VERITABLE SMORGASBORD OF
    “DIAGNOSES”
    When DSM‑5 was pub­lished in May 2013 it includ­ed some three hun­dred
    dis­or­ders in its 945 pages. It offers a ver­i­ta­ble smor­gas­bord of pos­si­ble
    labels for the prob­lems asso­ci­at­ed with severe ear­ly-life trau­ma, includ­ing
    some new ones such as Dis­rup­tive Mood Reg­u­la­tion Disorder,26 Non-
    sui­ci­dal Self Injury, Inter­mit­tent Explo­sive Dis­or­der, Dys­reg­u­lat­ed Social
    Engage­ment Dis­or­der, and Dis­rup­tive Impulse Con­trol Disorder.27
    Before the late nine­teenth cen­tu­ry doc­tors clas­si­fied ill­ness­es accord­ing
    to their sur­face man­i­fes­ta­tions, like fevers and pus­tules, which was not
    unrea­son­able, giv­en that they had lit­tle else to go on.28 This changed when
    sci­en­tists like Louis Pas­teur and Robert Koch dis­cov­ered that many
    dis­eases were caused by bac­te­ria that were invis­i­ble to the naked eye.
    Med­i­cine then was trans­formed by its attempts to dis­cov­er ways to get rid
    of those organ­isms rather than just treat­ing the boils and the fevers that they
    caused. With DSM‑5 psy­chi­a­try firm­ly regressed to ear­ly-nine­teenth-
    cen­tu­ry med­ical prac­tice. Despite the fact that we know the ori­gin of many
    of the prob­lems it iden­ti­fies, its “diag­noses” describe sur­face phe­nom­e­na
    that com­plete­ly ignore the under­ly­ing caus­es.
    Even before DSM‑5 was released, the Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Psy­chi­a­try
    pub­lished the results of valid­i­ty tests of var­i­ous new diag­noses, which
    indi­cat­ed that the DSM large­ly lacks what in the world of sci­ence is known
    as “reliability”—the abil­i­ty to pro­duce con­sis­tent, replic­a­ble results. In
    oth­er words, it lacks sci­en­tif­ic valid­i­ty. Odd­ly, the lack of reli­a­bil­i­ty and
    valid­i­ty did not keep the DSM‑5 from meet­ing its dead­line for pub­li­ca­tion,
    despite the near-uni­ver­sal con­sen­sus that it rep­re­sent­ed no improve­ment
    over the pre­vi­ous diag­nos­tic system.29 Could the fact that the APA had
    earned $100 mil­lion on the DSM-IV and is slat­ed to take in a sim­i­lar
    amount with the DSM‑5 (because all men­tal health prac­ti­tion­ers, many
    lawyers, and oth­er pro­fes­sion­als will be oblig­ed to pur­chase the lat­est
    edi­tion) be the rea­son we have this new diag­nos­tic sys­tem?
    Diag­nos­tic reli­a­bil­i­ty isn’t an abstract issue: If doc­tors can’t agree on
    what ails their patients, there is no way they can pro­vide prop­er treat­ment.
    When there’s no rela­tion­ship between diag­no­sis and cure, a mis­la­beled
    patient is bound to be a mis­treat­ed patient. You would not want to have your
    appen­dix removed when you are suf­fer­ing from a kid­ney stone, and you
    would not want have some­body labeled as “oppo­si­tion­al” when, in fact, his
    behav­ior is root­ed in an attempt to pro­tect him­self against real dan­ger.
    In a state­ment released in June 2011, the British Psy­cho­log­i­cal Soci­ety
    com­plained to the APA that the sources of psy­cho­log­i­cal suf­fer­ing in the
    DSM‑5 were iden­ti­fied “as locat­ed with­in indi­vid­u­als” and over­looked the
    “unde­ni­able social cau­sa­tion of many such problems.”30 This was in
    addi­tion to a flood of protest from Amer­i­can pro­fes­sion­als, includ­ing
    lead­ers of the Amer­i­can Psy­cho­log­i­cal Asso­ci­a­tion and the Amer­i­can
    Coun­sel­ing Asso­ci­a­tion. Why are rela­tion­ships or social con­di­tions left out?
    31 If you pay atten­tion only to faulty biol­o­gy and defec­tive genes as the
    cause of men­tal prob­lems and ignore aban­don­ment, abuse, and depri­va­tion,
    you are like­ly to run into as many dead ends as pre­vi­ous gen­er­a­tions did
    blam­ing it all on ter­ri­ble moth­ers.
    The most stun­ning rejec­tion of the DSM‑5 came from the Nation­al
    Insti­tute of Men­tal Health, which funds most psy­chi­atric research in
    Amer­i­ca. In April 2013, a few weeks before DSM‑5 was for­mal­ly released,
    NIMH direc­tor Thomas Insel announced that his agency could no longer
    sup­port DSM’s “symp­tom-based diagnosis.”32 Instead the insti­tute would
    focus its fund­ing on what are called Research Domain Cri­te­ria (RDoC)33 to
    cre­ate a frame­work for stud­ies that would cut across cur­rent diag­nos­tic
    cat­e­gories. For exam­ple, one of the NIMH domains is “Arousal/Modulatory
    Sys­tems (Arousal, Cir­ca­di­an Rhythm, Sleep and Wake­ful­ness),” which are
    dis­turbed to vary­ing degrees in many patients.
    Like the DSM‑5, the RDoC frame­work con­cep­tu­al­izes men­tal ill­ness­es
    sole­ly as brain dis­or­ders. This means that future research fund­ing will
    explore the brain cir­cuits “and oth­er neu­ro­bi­o­log­i­cal mea­sures” that
    under­lie men­tal prob­lems. Insel sees this as a first step toward the sort of
    “pre­ci­sion med­i­cine that has trans­formed can­cer diag­no­sis and treat­ment.”
    Men­tal ill­ness, how­ev­er, is not at all like can­cer: Humans are social
    ani­mals, and men­tal prob­lems involve not being able to get along with oth­er
    peo­ple, not fit­ting in, not belong­ing, and in gen­er­al not being able to get on
    the same wave­length.
    Every­thing about us—our brains, our minds, and our bodies—is geared
    toward col­lab­o­ra­tion in social sys­tems. This is our most pow­er­ful sur­vival
    strat­e­gy, the key to our suc­cess as a species, and it is pre­cise­ly this that
    breaks down in most forms of men­tal suf­fer­ing. As we saw in part 2, the
    neur­al con­nec­tions in brain and body are vital­ly impor­tant for
    under­stand­ing human suf­fer­ing, but it is impor­tant not to ignore the
    foun­da­tions of our human­i­ty: rela­tion­ships and inter­ac­tions that shape our
    minds and brains when we are young and that give sub­stance and mean­ing
    to our entire lives.
    Peo­ple with his­to­ries of abuse, neglect, or severe depri­va­tion will
    remain mys­te­ri­ous and large­ly untreat­ed unless we heed the admo­ni­tion of
    Alan Sroufe: “To ful­ly under­stand how we become the per­sons we are—the
    com­plex, step-by-step evo­lu­tion of our ori­en­ta­tions, capac­i­ties, and
    behav­ior over time—requires more than a list of ingre­di­ents, how­ev­er
    impor­tant any one of them might be. It requires an under­stand­ing of the
    process of devel­op­ment, how all of these fac­tors work togeth­er in an
    ongo­ing way over time.”34
    Front­line men­tal health workers—overwhelmed and under­paid social
    work­ers and ther­a­pists alike—seem to agree with our approach. Short­ly
    after the APA reject­ed Devel­op­men­tal Trau­ma Dis­or­der for inclu­sion in the
    DSM, thou­sands of clin­i­cians from around the coun­try sent small
    con­tri­bu­tions to the Trau­ma Cen­ter to help us con­duct a large sci­en­tif­ic
    study, known as a field tri­al, to fur­ther study DTD. That sup­port has
    enabled us to inter­view hun­dreds of kids, par­ents, fos­ter par­ents, and men­tal
    health work­ers at five dif­fer­ent net­work sites over the last few years with
    sci­en­tif­i­cal­ly con­struct­ed inter­view tools. The first results from these
    stud­ies have now been pub­lished, and more will appear as this book is
    going to print.35
    WHAT DIFFERENCE WOULD DTD MAKE?
    One answer is that it would focus research and treat­ment (not to men­tion
    fund­ing) on the cen­tral prin­ci­ples that under­lie the pro­tean symp­toms of
    chron­i­cal­ly trau­ma­tized chil­dren and adults: per­va­sive bio­log­i­cal and
    emo­tion­al dys­reg­u­la­tion, failed or dis­rupt­ed attach­ment, prob­lems stay­ing
    focused and on track, and a huge­ly defi­cient sense of coher­ent per­son­al
    iden­ti­ty and com­pe­tence. These issues tran­scend and include almost all
    diag­nos­tic cat­e­gories, but treat­ment that doesn’t put them front and cen­ter is
    more than like­ly to miss the mark. Our great chal­lenge is to apply the
    lessons of neu­ro­plas­tic­i­ty, the flex­i­bil­i­ty of brain cir­cuits, to rewire the
    brains and reor­ga­nize the minds of peo­ple who have been pro­grammed by
    life itself to expe­ri­ence oth­ers as threats and them­selves as help­less.
    Social sup­port is a bio­log­i­cal neces­si­ty, not an option, and this real­i­ty
    should be the back­bone of all pre­ven­tion and treat­ment. Rec­og­niz­ing the
    pro­found effects of trau­ma and depri­va­tion on child devel­op­ment need not
    lead to blam­ing par­ents. We can assume that par­ents do the best they can,
    but all par­ents need help to nur­ture their kids. Near­ly every indus­tri­al­ized
    nation, with the excep­tion of the Unit­ed States, rec­og­nizes this and pro­vides
    some form of guar­an­teed sup­port to fam­i­lies. James Heck­man, win­ner of
    the 2000 Nobel Prize in Eco­nom­ics, has shown that qual­i­ty ear­ly-child­hood
    pro­grams that involve par­ents and pro­mote basic skills in dis­ad­van­taged
    chil­dren more than pay for them­selves in improved outcomes.36
    In the ear­ly 1970s psy­chol­o­gist David Olds was work­ing in a Bal­ti­more
    day-care cen­ter where many of the preschool­ers came from homes wracked
    by pover­ty, domes­tic vio­lence, and drug abuse. Aware that only address­ing
    the children’s prob­lems at school was not suf­fi­cient to improve their home
    con­di­tions, he start­ed a home-vis­i­ta­tion pro­gram in which skilled nurs­es
    helped moth­ers to pro­vide a safe and stim­u­lat­ing envi­ron­ment for their
    chil­dren and, in the process, to imag­ine a bet­ter future for them­selves.
    Twen­ty years lat­er, the chil­dren of the home-vis­i­ta­tion moth­ers were not
    only health­i­er but also less like­ly to report hav­ing been abused or neglect­ed
    than a sim­i­lar group whose moth­ers had not been vis­it­ed. They also were
    more like­ly to have fin­ished school, to have stayed out of jail, and to be
    work­ing in well-pay­ing jobs. Econ­o­mists have cal­cu­lat­ed that every dol­lar
    invest­ed in high-qual­i­ty home vis­i­ta­tion, day care, and preschool pro­grams
    results in sev­en dol­lars of sav­ings on wel­fare pay­ments, health-care costs,
    sub­stance-abuse treat­ment, and incar­cer­a­tion, plus high­er tax rev­enues due
    to bet­ter-pay­ing jobs.37
    When I go to Europe to teach, I often am con­tact­ed by offi­cials at the
    min­istries of health in the Scan­di­na­vian coun­tries, the Unit­ed King­dom,
    Ger­many, or the Nether­lands and asked to spend an after­noon with them
    shar­ing the lat­est research on the treat­ment of trau­ma­tized chil­dren,
    ado­les­cents, and their fam­i­lies. The same is true for many of my col­leagues.
    These coun­tries have already made a com­mit­ment to uni­ver­sal health care,
    ensur­ing a guar­an­teed min­i­mum wage, paid parental leave for both par­ents
    after a child is born, and high-qual­i­ty child­care for all work­ing moth­ers.

    0 Comments

    Heads up! Your comment will be invisible to other guests and subscribers (except for replies), including you after a grace period.
    Note